Intelligence testing began in earnest in France when, in 1904, psychologist Alfred Binet was commissioned by the French government to find a method to differentiate between intellectually normal children and those inferior. The purpose was to put the latter into special schools where they would receive more individual attention. In this way, the disruption they caused in the education of intellectually normal children could be avoided.1
This led to the development of the Binet Scale, also known as the Simon-Binet Scale, in recognition of Theophile Simon’s assistance in its development. It constituted a revolutionary approach to the assessment of individual mental ability. However, Binet himself cautioned against misuse of the scale or misunderstanding of its implications. According to Binet, the scale was designed with a single purpose in mind; it was to serve as a guide to identifying children in the schools who required special education. Its intention was not to be used as “a general device for ranking all pupils according to mental worth.”
Binet also noted that “the scale, properly speaking, does not permit the measure of intelligence, because intellectual qualities are not superposable, and therefore cannot be measured as linear surfaces are measured.”2 Since, according to Binet, intelligence could not be described as a single score, the use of the Intelligence Quotient (IQ) as a definite statement of a child’s intellectual capability would be a serious mistake. In addition, Binet feared that IQ measurement would be used to condemn a child to a permanent “condition” of stupidity, thereby negatively affecting their education and livelihood:
Binet’s scale had a profound impact on educational development in the United States — and elsewhere. However, the American educators and psychologists who championed and utilized the scale and its revisions failed to heed Binet’s caveats concerning its limitations. Soon, intelligence testing assumed importance and respectability out of proportion to its actual value.
H. H. Goddard, director of research at Vineland Training School in New Jersey, translated Binet’s work into English and advocated a more general application of the Simon-Binet Scale.4 Unlike Binet, Goddard considered intelligence a solitary, fixed, and inborn entity that could be measured.5
While Goddard extolled the value and uses of the single IQ score, Lewis M. Terman, who also believed that intelligence was hereditary and fixed, worked on revising the Simon-Binet Scale. His final product, published in 1916 as the Stanford Revision of the Binet-Simon Scale of Intelligence (also known as the Stanford-Binet), became the standard intelligence test in the United States for the next several decades.6 Once American educators had been convinced of the need for universal intelligence testing and the efficiency it could contribute to school programming, within a few years,
Few people realize that the tests being used today — of which the IQ test continues to be the most popular — represent the end result of a historical process that has its origins in racial and cultural bigotry. Many of the founding fathers of the modern testing industry — including Goddard, Terman, and Carl Brighan (the developer of the Scholastic Aptitude Test) — advocated eugenics.8
Eugenics is a movement concerned with the selective breeding of human beings. Selected human beings would be mated with each other in an attempt to obtain certain traits in their offspring, much the same way that animal breeders work with champion stock. The eventual goal of eugenics is to create a better human race. The Nazis took this idea to the extreme. All “inferior” humans, especially Jews, intellectually disabled children or adults, and any individuals with genetic defects, were to be destroyed; and so many ill and intellectually disabled people, and many Jews, were killed during World War II.9
The founding fathers of the testing industry saw testing as one way of achieving the eugenicist aims. Goddard’s belief in the innateness and inalterability of intelligence levels, for example, was so firm that he argued for the reconstruction of society along the lines dictated by IQ scores:
As a result of his views on intelligence and society, Goddard lobbied for restrictive immigration laws. Upon his “discovery” that all immigrants, except those from Northern Europe, were of “surprisingly low intelligence,” tight immigration laws were enacted in the 1920s.11 According to Harvard professor Steven Jay Gould in his acclaimed book The Mismeasure of Man, these tests were also influential in legitimizing forced sterilization of allegedly “defective” individuals in some states.12
By the 1920s, mass use of the Stanford-Binet Scale and other tests had created a multimillion-dollar testing industry.13 By 1974, according to the Mental Measurements Yearbook, 2,467 tests measuring some form of intellectual ability were in print, 76 of which were identified as strict intelligence tests.14 In one year in the 1980s, teachers gave over 500 million standardized tests to children and adults across the United States.15 In 1989, the American Academy for the Advancement of Science listed the IQ test among the twenty most significant scientific discoveries of the twentieth century, along with nuclear fission, DNA, the transistor, and flight.16 Patricia Broadfoot’s dictum that “assessment, far more than religion, has become the opiate of the people,”17 has come of age.
So what are we actually measuring?
If an IQ test is supposed to measure a person’s intelligence, the question is: What is intelligence? Is it the ability to do well in school? Is it the ability to read well and spell correctly? Or are the following people intelligent?
- The physician who smokes three packets of cigarettes a day?
- The Nobel Prize winner whose marriage and personal life are in ruins?
- The corporate executive who has ingeniously worked his way to the top and also earned a heart attack for his efforts?
- The brilliant and successful music composer who handled his money so poorly that he was always running from his creditors (incidentally, his name was Mozart)?18
.
The problem is that the term intelligence has never been defined adequately; therefore, nobody knows what an IQ test should measure. Despite this, the results of this test determine thousands of children’s futures.
Already in the early 1920s, the journalist Walter Lippmann maintained that IQ tests were nothing but a series of stunts. “We cannot measure intelligence when we have not defined it,” he said.19
In 1962, Banesh Hoffman told a shocked America about the “tyranny of testing” in his classic book of the same name. His book and others that followed stirred up much controversy, leading the National Education Association in 1976 to recommend the elimination of group standardized intelligence, aptitude, and achievement tests.20 Sarason quotes an advertisement that was placed by Psychology Today in the New York Times in August 1979, part of which appears below:
The National Education Association, with a membership of almost 2 million teachers, has called for the abolition of standardized intelligence tests because they are “at best wasteful, and at worst, destructive.”
Yale psychologist Robert Sternberg says in P.T. that psychologists know “almost nothing about what it is that they have been measuring. The tests have proved overall to have only low to moderate power to predict such things as future job performance, income and status, or overall happiness and adjustment.”21
However, the dust soon settled after this uprising, and the testing industry became more powerful than ever. The National Education Association has wholly changed its stand and now “recognizes the need for periodic comprehensive testing for evaluation and diagnosis of student progress.”22 This is no wonder, says Dr. Thomas Armstrong, since it would have taken a major miracle to eliminate testing.23
Today, voices for the elimination of standardized tests are few. One is Linda S. Siegel, aprofessor in the Department of Educational Psychology and Special Education at the University of British Columbia in Vancouver, Canada. She proposed that we abandon the IQ test in the analysis of the learning-disabled child. According to most definitions — although they are not conclusive — intelligence is made up of the skills of logical reasoning, problem-solving, critical thinking, and adaptation.24 This scenario seems reasonable until one examines the content of IQ tests. The definition of intelligence, as is operationalized in all IQ tests, includes virtually no skills that can be identified in terms of the definitions of intelligence.
To support her statement, Siegel gives a detailed analysis of the subtests of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R). This IQ test is composed of Verbal and Performance sections and is nearly always used in learning disabilities (LD) diagnoses. In each subtest of the Verbal scale, performance is in varying degrees dependent on specific knowledge, vocabulary, expressive language, and memory skills. In the Performance scale, visual-spatial abilities, fine motor coordination, perceptual skills, and, in some subtests, speed are essential for scoring.25 As Siegel rightly points out, IQ tests measure, for the most part, what a person has learned, not what they are capable of doing in the future (his potential).26
There is an additional problem with using IQ tests for individuals with learning disabilities. According to Siegel, it is a paradox that IQ scores are required of people with LD because most have deficiencies in one or more of the component skills that are part of these IQ tests — memory, language, fine motor skills, et cetera. The effect is that they may have a lower IQ score than a person who does not have such problems, even though they may both have identical reasoning and problem-solving skills. The lower IQ score, therefore, may result from the learning disability, and IQ scores may underestimate the real intelligence of the individual with a learning disability.27
Another assumption of the discrepancy definition is that the IQ score should predict reading, so if you have a low IQ score, you should be a poor reader, and poor reading is an expected consequence of low IQ. However, there are individuals with low IQ scores and are good readers.28
Numerous researchers have proven the unreliability of IQ tests. The scores may vary by as much as 15 points from one test to another,29 while emotional tension, anxiety, and unfamiliarity with the testing process can greatly affect test performance.30 In addition, Gould described the biasing effect that tester attitudes, qualifications, and instructions can have on testing.31 In one study, for example, ninety-nine school psychologists independently scored an IQ test from identical records and came up with IQs ranging from 63 to 117 for the same person.32
In another study, Ysseldyke et al. examined the extent to which professionals were able to differentiate learning-disabled students from ordinary low achievers by examining patterns of scores on psychometric measures. Subjects were 65 school psychologists, 38 special-education teachers, and a “naive” group of 21 university students enrolled in programs unrelated to education or psychology. Provided with forms containing information on 41 test or subtest scores (including the WISC-R IQ test) of nine school-identified LD students and nine non-LD students, judges were instructed to indicate which students they believed were learning disabled and which were non-learning disabled.33
The school psychologists and special-education teachers were able to differentiate between LD students and low achievers with only 50 percent accuracy. The naive judges, who had never had more than an introductory course in education or psychology, evidenced a 75 percent hit rate.34 When Ysseldyke and Algozzine cite Scriven, they clearly show their belief that the current system is in trouble:
.
References:
- Swiegers, D. J., & Louw, D. A., “Intelligensie,” in D. A. Louw (ed.), Inleiding tot die Psigologie (2nd ed.), (Johannesburg: McGraw Hill, 1982), 145.
- Gould, S. J., The Mismeasure of Man (New York: W. W. Norton, 1981), 151-152, cited in Osgood, “Intelligence testing and the field of learning disabilities: A historical and critical perspective,” Learning Disability Quarterly, 1984, vol. 7, 343-348.
- Gould, The Mismeasure of Man, 153-154, cited in Osgood, “Intelligence testing.”
- Gould, The Mismeasure of Man, 159, cited in Osgood, “Intelligence testing.”
- Goddard, H. H., Human Efficiency and Levels of Intelligence (Princeton: Princeton University Press, 1920), 1, cited in Osgood, “Intelligence testing.”
- Linden, K. W., & Linden, J. D., Modern Mental Measurement: A Historical Perspective(Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1968), cited in Osgood, “Intelligence testing.”
- Osgood, “Intelligence testing.”
- Armstrong, T., In Their Own Way: Discovering and Encouraging Your Child’s Personal Learning Style (Los Angeles: Jeremy P. Tarcher, Inc., 1987), 27.
- Dworetzky, J. P., Introduction to Child Development (St. Paul: West Publishing Company, 1981), 82-83.
- Goddard, Human Efficiency and Levels of Intelligence, v-vii, cited in Osgood, “Intelligence testing.”
- Gould, The Mismeasure of Man, 167, cited in Osgood, “Intelligence testing.”
- Gould, The Mismeasure of Man, cited in Armstrong, In Their Own Way, 28.
- Osgood, “Intelligence testing.”
- Buros, O. K. (ed.), Mental Measurements Yearbook (Highland Park, NJ: Gryphon Press), cited in Osgood, “Intelligence testing.”
- Armstrong, In Their Own Way, 27.
- Bjorklund, D. F., Children’s Thinking: Development Function and Individual Differences(Pacific Grove, CA: Brookes/Cole, 1989), cited in P. Engelbrecht, S. Kriegler & M. Booysen (eds.), Perspectives on Learning Difficulties (Pretoria: J. L. van Schaik, 1996), 109.
- Broadfoot, P., cited in Engelbrecht et al. (eds.), Perspectives on Learning Difficulties, 109.
- Dworetzky, Introduction to Child Development, 348.
- Lippman, cited in N. J Block & G. Dworkin (eds.), The IQ Controversy: Critical Readings(New York: Pantheon Books, 1976).
- Armstrong, In Their Own Way, 26.
- New York Times, August 1979, cited in S. B. Sarason, Psychology Misdirected (New York: The Free Press, 1981).
- National Education Association Handbook, 1984-85 (Washington, DC: National Education Association of the United States, 1984, 240), cited in Armstrong, In Their Own Way, 27.
- Armstrong, In Their Own Way, 27.
- Siegel, L. S., “Issues in the definition and diagnosis of learning disabilities: A perspective on Guckenberger v. Boston University,” Journal of Learning Disabilities, 1 July 1999, vol. 32.
- Siegel, L. S., “IQ is irrelevant to the definition of learning disabilities,” Journal of Learning Disabilities, 1989, vol. 22(8), 469-478.
- Siegel, “Issues in the definition and diagnosis of learning disabilities.”
- Ibid; Siegel, “IQ is irrelevant to the definition of learning disabilities.”
- Siegel, L. S., & Metsala, E., “An alternative to the food processor approach to subtypes of learning disabilities,” in N. N. Singh & I. L. Beale (eds.), Learning Disabilities: Nature, Theory, and Treatment (New York: Springler-Verlag, 1992), 45.
- Smith, C. R., Learning Disabilities: The Interaction of Learner, Task, and Setting(Boston: Allyn and Bacon, 1991), 63.
- Tyler, cited in A. Anastasi, (ed.), Testing Problems in Perspective (Washington DC: American Council on Education, 1966).
- Gould, The Mismeasure of Man, 199-212, cited in Osgood, “Intelligence testing.”
- Cited in J. Sattler, Assessment of Children’s Intelligences and Special Abilities (Boston: Allyn & Bacon, 1982), 60.
- Epps, S., Ysseldyke, J. E., & McGue, M., “’I know one when I see one’—Differentiating LD and non-LD students,” Learning Disability Quarterly, 1984, vol. 7, 89-101.
- Ysseldyke, J. E., & Algozzine, B., “LD or not LD: That’s not the question!” Journal of Learning Disabilities, 1983, vol. 16(1), 26-27.
- Scriven, M., “Comments on Gene Glass,” Paper presented at the Wingspread National Invitational Conference on Public Policy and the Special Education Task of the 1980s, cited in Ysseldyke & Algozzine, “LD or not LD.”